Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schema and the processing of social information. Psychol. Bull. 112, 461–484. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.3.461 Beier, J. S., Over, H., and Carpenter, M. (2014). Young children help others to achieve their social goals. Dev. Psychol. 50, 934. doi: 10.1037/a0033886 Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. Middlesex: Pelican Books. [Original work published 1969]. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., and Shaver, P. R. (1998). “Self-report measurement of adult romantic attachment: an integrative overview,” in Attachment Theory and Close Relationships, eds J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (New York: Guilford Press), 46–76. Brewer, M. B., and Caporael, L. R. (2006). “An evolutionary perspective on social identity: Revisiting groups.” In Evolution and social psychology, eds M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, and D. T. Kendrick (New York: Psychology Press), 143–161. Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (2008). Handbook of Attachment, Second Edition: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New York: Oxford University Press. Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 337–367. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.337 De Wolff, M. S., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedent of infant attachment. Child Dev. 68, 571–591. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.x Dunfield, K. A. (2014). A construct divided: prosocial behaviour as helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes. Front. Psychol. 5:958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00958 Dunfield, K. A., and Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes. Child Dev. 84, 1766–1776. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12075 Dunfield, K. A., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O’Connell, L. J., and Kelley, E. A. (2011). Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy. Infancy 16, 227–247. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.x Dweck, C. S. (2013). “Social-cognitive development,” in Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Teach Us, eds M. R. Banaji and S. G. German (New York: Oxford University Press), 3–5. Dykas, M. J., and Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: theory and evidence. Psychol. Bull. 137, 19–46. doi: 10.1037/a0021367 Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., and Knafo, A. (2015). “Prosocial development,” in Handbook of Child Psychology, 7th Edn, eds M. Lamb and C. Garcia-Coll, Vol. ed. R. M. Lerner (New York: Wiley), 610–656. Fawcett, C., and Liszkowski, U. (2012). Infants anticipate others’ social preferences. Infant. Child. Deve. 21, 239–249. Guastella, A. J., Mitchell, P. B., and Dadds, M. R. (2008). Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 3–5. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.026 Hamlin, J. K. (2014). The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: gazing toward one’s goal drives infants’ preferences for Helpers over Hinderers in the hill paradigm. Front. Psychol. 5:1563. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01563 Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: evidence for an innate moral core. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 186–193. doi: 10.1177/0963721412470687 Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., and Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450, 557–559. doi: 10.1038/nature06288 Hobbs, K., and Spelke, E. (2015). Goal attributions and instrumental helping at 14 and 24 months of age. Cognition 142, 44–59. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.014 Johnson, S. C., and Chen, F. S. (2011). Socioemotional information processing in human infants: from genes to subjective construals. Emot. Rev. 3, 169–178. doi: 10.1177/1754073910387945 Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C., and Chen, F. S. (2007). Evidence for infants’ internal working model of attachment. Psychol. Sci. 18, 501–502. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01929.x Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C., Chen, F. S., Ok, S. J., Stern, H. L., and Barth, M. E. (2010). At the intersection of social and cognitive development: internal working models of attachment in infancy. Cogn. Sci. 34, 807–825. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01112.x Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S., and Dunfield, K. A. (2013). “How universals and individual differences can inform each other: the case for social expectations in infancy,” in Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Teach Us, eds M. R. Banaji and S. G. German (New York: Oxford University Press), 44–48. Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., and Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychol. Sci. 14, 402–408. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01454 Main, M., Kaplan, N., and Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: a move to the level of representation. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 50, 66–104. doi: 10.2307/3333827 Oakes, L. M. (2010). Using habituation of looking time to assess mental processes in infancy. J. Cogn. Dev. 11, 255–268. doi: 10.1080/15248371003699977 Olson, K. R., and Dweck, C. S. (2008). A blueprint for social cognitive development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 193–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00074.x Olson, K. R., and Dweck, C. S. (2009). Social cognitive development: a new look. Child Dev. Perspect. 3, 60–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00078.x Paulus, M., Kühn-Popp, N., Licata, M., Sodian, B., and Meinhardt, J. (2013). Neural correlates of prosocial behavior in infancy: different neurophysiological mechanisms support the emergence of helping and comforting. Neuroimage 66, 522–530. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.041 Piaget, J. (1995). Logical Operations and Social Life. New York: Routledge. [Original work published in 1945]. Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants attribute value ± to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 848–856. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.848 Spelke, E. S. (1985). “Preferential looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy,” in Measurement of Audition and Vision in the First Year of Postnatal Life, eds G. Gottlieb and N. Krasnegor (Norwood, NJ: Ablex), 323–363. Spelke, E. S. (2014). Core social cognition. Paper Presented at the Concordia Science College Public Lecture Series, Montreal, QC. Svetlova, M., Nichols, S. R., and Brownell, C. A. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: from instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Dev. 81, 1814–1827. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x Van Overwalle, F., and Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 48, 564–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009 Vygotsky, L. (1978). “Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes,” eds M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, and E. Souberman (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press). Warneken, F., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy 11, 271–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.x Warneken, F., and Tomasello, M. (2009). Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 397–402. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.008 Woodward, A. L., Sommerville, J. A., and Guajardo, J. J. (2001). “How infants make sense of intentional action,” in Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition, eds B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, and D. A. Baldwin (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 149–169. Yamaguchi, M., Kuhlmeier, V. A., Wynn, K., and VanMarle, K. (2009). Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood. Dev. Sci. 12, 746–752. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x