Gómez Díaz, Miranda ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1503-6893 (2024) Investigating the Vocabulary Spurt in Bilingual and Monolingual Infants. Masters thesis, Concordia University.
Preview |
Text (application/pdf)
1MBGomezDiaz_MA_F2024.pdf - Accepted Version Available under License Spectrum Terms of Access. |
Abstract
Sometime before their second birthday, many children have a period of rapid expressive vocabulary growth called the vocabulary spurt. Theories of the underlying mechanisms differ: accumulator models emphasize the accumulation of experience with words over time to yield a spurt-like pattern, while cognitive models attribute the spurt to cognitive changes. To test these theories, English–French monolingual and bilingual children with different exposure to each language were studied. Dense, longitudinal data was analyzed from 45 infants aged 16-30 months, whose expressive vocabulary was measured on a total of 617 occasions in English and/or French. Single-language (English and/or French), concept (number of concepts lexicalized across both languages), and word (sum of both languages) vocabulary scores were computed. Infants’ exposure to each language and their exposure balance were measured using a language exposure questionnaire. Logistic curves were fitted to each infant’s data to estimate the timing (midpoint) and steepness (slope) of the vocabulary spurt in single-language, concept, and word vocabularies. 76% of infants showed a spurt in at least one vocabulary type, and bilinguals were less likely to show one in their non-dominant than their dominant language. For single-language vocabulary, infants with more exposure to a language had earlier spurts. For combined vocabularies (concept and word), monolinguals and unbalanced bilinguals had earlier and steeper spurts than balanced bilinguals. Results better support the predictions of accumulator models than cognitive theories, and show that infants follow different vocabulary acquisition trajectories based on their language background.
Divisions: | Concordia University > Faculty of Arts and Science > Psychology |
---|---|
Item Type: | Thesis (Masters) |
Authors: | Gómez Díaz, Miranda |
Institution: | Concordia University |
Degree Name: | M.A. |
Program: | Psychology |
Date: | April 2024 |
Thesis Supervisor(s): | Byers-Heinlein, Krista |
Keywords: | infant bilingualism, vocabulary spurt, language acquisition, vocabulary, longitudinal study |
ID Code: | 993868 |
Deposited By: | Miranda Gomez Diaz |
Deposited On: | 24 Oct 2024 18:58 |
Last Modified: | 24 Oct 2024 18:58 |
References:
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6(2), 183-200. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900002245Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 3253-3258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113380109
Bilson, S., Yoshida, H., Tran, C. D., Woods, E. A., & Hills, T. T. (2015). Semantic facilitation in bilingual first language acquisition. Cognition, 140(C), 122–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.013
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2(1), 29-49. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0201_3
Boudreault, M.-C., Cabirol, É.-A., Trudeau, N., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Sutton, A. (2007). Les Inventaires Macarthur du développement de la communication: validité et données normatives préliminaires [MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: Validity and preliminary normative data]. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 31(1), 27-37. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2009-09640-003
Byers-Heinlein, K., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., Schott, E., & Killam, H. (2023). Sometimes larger, sometimes smaller: Measuring vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual infants and toddlers. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x7s4u
Byers-Heinlein, K., Schott, E., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., Brouillard, M., Dubé, D., Jardak, A., Laoun-Rubenstein, A., Mastroberardino, M., Morin-Lessard, E., Pour Iliaei, S., Salama-Siroishka, N., & Tamayo, M. P. (2020). MAPLE: A multilingual approach to parent language estimates. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 951-957. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000282
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition, 128(3), 407-416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.010
Collisson, B. A., Graham, S. A., Preston, J. L., Rose, M. S., McDonald, S., & Tough, S. (2016). Risk and protective factors for late talking: An epidemiologic investigation. The Journal of Pediatrics, 172, 168-174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.02.020
DeAnda, S., Arias-Trejo, N., Poulin-Dubois, D., Zesiger, P., & Friend, M. (2016). Minimal second language exposure, SES, and early word comprehension: New evidence from a direct assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 162-180. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000820
deMayo, B., Kellier, D., Braginsky, M., Bergmann, C., Hendriks, C., Rowland, C. F., Frank, M. C., & Marchman, V. (2021). Web-CDI: A system for online administration of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. Language Development Research, 1(1), 55-98. https://doi.org/10.34842/kr8e-w591
D'Odorico, L., Carubbi, S., Salerni, N., & Calvo, V. (2001). Vocabulary development in Italian children: A longitudinal evaluation of quantitative and qualitative aspects. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 351-372. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000901004676
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, Tomasello, M., Mervis, C. B., & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), i-185. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166093
Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., & Marchman, V. A. (2021). Variability and consistency in early language learning: The Wordbank project. MIT Press.
Ganger, J., & Brent, M. R. (2004). Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 621–632. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.621
Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17(1), 171–183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900013167
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The development of categorization in the second year and its relations to other cognitive and linguistic developments. Child Development, 58(6),1523–1531. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130692
Hidaka, S. (2013). A computational model associating learning process, word attributes, and age of acquisition. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e76242. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076242
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000759
Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., Burridge, A., Ribot, K. M., & Welsh, S. N. (2014). Expressive vocabulary development in children from bilingual and monolingual homes: A longitudinal study from two to four years. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 433-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.012
Kachergis, G., Marchman, V. A., & Frank, M. C. (2022). Toward a “standard model” of early language learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(1), 20-27. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211057836
Kalashnikova, M., Mattock, K., & Monaghan, P. (2016). Mutual exclusivity develops as a consequence of abstract rather than particular vocabulary knowledge. First Language, 36(5), 451-464. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716648850
Kauschke, C., & Hofmeister, C. (2002). Early lexical development in German: A study on vocabulary growth and vocabulary composition during the second and third year of life. Journal of Child Language, 29(4), 735-757. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005330
Kircher, R. (2014). Thirty Years after Bill 101: A Contemporary Perspective on Attitudes Towards English and French in Montreal. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 17(1), 20-50.
Kremin, L. V., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2021). Why not both? Rethinking categorical and continuous approaches to bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(6), 1560-1575. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069211031986
Longoria, A. Q., Page, M. C., Hubbs-Tait, L., & Kennison, S. M. (2009). Relationship between kindergarten children's language ability and social competence. Early Child Development and Care, 179(7), 919–929. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430701590241
Mahr, T.J. (2019, February 15). Anatomy of a logistic growth curve. Higher Order Functions. https://www.tjmahr.com/anatomy-of-a-logistic-growth-curve/
Marchman, V. A., Dale, P. S., Fenson, L. (2023). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual (3rd ed.). Brookes Publishing.
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9-F16. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.x
Marchman, V. A., Martínez, L. Z., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A. (2017). Caregiver talk to young Spanish‐English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent‐report measures of dual‐language exposure. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12425. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12425
Marchman, V. A., & Martínez-Sussmann, C. (2002). Concurrent validity of caregiver/parent report measures of language for children who are learning both English and Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5), 983-997. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/080)
McMurray, B. (2007). Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science, 317(5838), 631. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144073
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the Novel Name-Nameless Category (N3C) Principle. Child Development, 65(6), 1646–1662. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131285
Orena, A. J., Byers‐Heinlein, K., & Polka, L. (2020). What do bilingual infants actually hear? Evaluating measures of language input to bilingual‐learning 10‐month‐olds. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12901. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12901
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93-120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x
Posit team (2023). RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Posit Software, PBC, Boston, MA. http://www.posit.co/
Poulin-Dubois, D., Graham, S., & Sippola, L. (1995). Early lexical development: The contribution of parental labelling and infants' categorization abilities. Journal of Child Language, 22(2), 325-343. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900009818
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/
Ramírez‐Esparza, N., García‐Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2014). Look who's talking: Speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17(6), 880-891. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12172
Ramírez‐Esparza, N., García‐Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). The impact of early social interactions on later language development in Spanish–English bilingual infants. Child Development, 88(4), 1216-1234. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12648
Rescorla, L. (2011). Late talkers: Do good predictors of outcome exist? Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 17(2), 141-150. https://doi.org/10.1002/ddrr.1108
Rescorla, L., Mirak, J., & Singh, L. (2000). Vocabulary growth in late talkers: Lexical development from 2; 0 to 3; 0. Journal of Child Language, 27(2), 293-311. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090000413X
Reznick, J. S., & Goldfield, B. A. (1992). Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 406–413. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.28.3.406
Ritz, C., Baty, F., Streibig, J. C., Gerhard, D. (2015) Dose-Response Analysis Using R. PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0146021. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0146021
Silvén, M., Voeten, M., Kouvo, A., & Lunden, M. (2014). Speech perception and vocabulary growth: A longitudinal study of Finnish-Russian bilinguals and Finnish monolinguals from infancy to three years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(4), 323-332. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1177/0165025414533748
Statistics Canada. (2023). Low income cut-offs (LICOs) before and after tax by community size and family size, in constant dollars [Table 11-10-0195-01]. https://doi.org/10.25318/1110019501-eng
Suttora, C., Guarini, A., Zuccarini, M., Aceti, A., Corvaglia, L., & Sansavini, A. (2020). Speech and language skills of low-risk preterm and full-term late talkers: The role of child factors and parent input. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(20), 7684. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17207684
Tan, A. W., Marchman, V. A., & Frank, M. C. (2024). The role of translation equivalents in bilingual word learning. Developmental Science, e13476. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13476
Tsui, R. K. Y., Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., Schott, E., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2022). Are translation equivalents special? Evidence from simulations and empirical data from bilingual infants. Cognition, 225, 105084. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105084
Trudeau, N., & Sutton, A. (2011). Expressive vocabulary and early grammar of 16-to 30-month-old children acquiring Quebec French. First Language, 31(4), 480-507. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723711410828
Turcotte, M. (2019). Results from the 2016 census: English-French bilingualism among Canadian children and youth. Statistics Canada. https://epe.lac-bac.gc.ca/100/201/301/weekly_acquisitions_list-ef/2019/19-40/publications.gc.ca/collections/collection_2019/statcan/75-006-x/75-006-2019-14-eng.pdf
Repository Staff Only: item control page